|
|
Subject:
Statistics on the Psychology of Noticing Things.
Category: Miscellaneous Asked by: shapai-ga List Price: $10.00 |
Posted:
15 Jun 2006 21:27 PDT
Expires: 15 Jul 2006 21:27 PDT Question ID: 738603 |
We have heard that if something is left unchanged for long periods that people tend to develop a blind spot for it and not notice it anymore. eg. With Licence (number) plates on cars, we don?t tend to notice them unless people add something to them, there may be a colour change of the Licence (number) plates, or people may put a colourful frame around them and suddenly our eyes gravitate towards what we took no notice of in the past. Although the Licence (number) plates were all around us before, we didn?t ?pay attention? to them until there was a slight change. (The overall appearance of the original design stayed the same, though the addition of the frame brought our attention to them) (It is basically trying to prove that the house you have never noticed on your street suddenly gets painted blue, you then notice it every time you drive past it.) We would like to find official statistics from Government sources or reputable market researchers/professors etc., not just statistics from a magazine or newspaper. We want to prove 2 things: a) We want statistical proof that the more something is in front of your face, the more likely they are not to see it, or not even realize it is there. The fact that if something is a constant and ?left completely unchanged? for long periods then people don?t notice it anymore, even though it may be all around us. b) We want statistical evidence that if you keep the original design of something, but add something to it (even only slight changes), that it becomes more noticeable. People tend to notice it (for the first time, or again) even though it was right in front of them the whole time. (If you come across any information on ?what period of time? it takes people to ?not notice? something between any physical changes, that would be great also, eg. after 2 months people develop a blind spot etc.) Thank you very much. |
|
There is no answer at this time. |
|
Subject:
Re: Statistics on the Psychology of Noticing Things.
From: pademelon-ga on 25 Jun 2006 05:51 PDT |
Hullo shapai-ga, maybe I can start the ball rolling. Consider the puzzle of new speed limit signs... have you ever bumbled into an area where the speed limit has been reduced and not seen the changed sign ? Women are much, much, better at noticing things.. such as what's buried at the back of the 'fridge and lipstick smudges. |
Subject:
Re: Statistics on the Psychology of Noticing Things.
From: myoarin-ga on 25 Jun 2006 07:54 PDT |
I am surprised that there hasn't till now been a response to your interesting question. The problem may be the request for "statistical proof" and not just learned papers on the subject. I believe the premise is entirely correct, relating to visual memory, selective visual memory, and some built-in efficiencies in the system, which I equate to the way I understand (maybe incorrectly) that digitalized moving images are compacted by only recording changes from one monitor image to the next. The example of the blue house could be straight from my experience. There is a street that I often drive through, but since in never stop in it, all I can recall specifically is that I pass a church and hairdresser's sign and a side street - until one non-descript house was indeed painted blue, which now catches my eye every time, even after several years. In our everyday work and home environment, all of us have stored image of things and immediately recognize a missing, additional or moved item (not that that always makes it easier to find something). I have read that Australian Aboriginal trackers don't so much see tracks - foot or paw prints - but recognize disturbances in the terrain, having a well-trained general visual image of what undisturbed terrain should look like. I have also read that women are thought to have a better memory for things - now presumably genetically coded - since in the days of hunters and gatherers, they were the latter and learned to recall where plants grew and to recognize them. But this is not entirely true: once a woman who knew me only with a beard, greeted me and rode the elevator with me without noticing that I had shaved it off, only recognizing the fact when someone else later mentioned it to her. Similarly, once when I parted my hair on the other side, only one person noticed immediately. It's a very interesting subject. I hope someone adds some professional links. |
Subject:
Re: Statistics on the Psychology of Noticing Things.
From: atg77401-ga on 03 Jul 2006 06:58 PDT |
For part B, the phenomenon you are looking to research not actual visual memory, but visual perception (I know they sound the same, but they are very very different. The phenomenon is generally referred to in psychological literature as "feature presence" versus "feature absence." Specifcally, When you present a letter sheet, such as XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX XXXXXXXXXXXPXXXX XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX XXXXXXXXXXXXXXXX it is easy to spot the "P", because the feature "roundness" or "o-ness" is additionally present. It results in what is commonly called "parallel popout" as the P seems to pop out and is clearly visable at first glance. However, with feature absence: RRRRRRRRRRRRRRRR RRRRRRRRRRRRRRRR RRRPRRRRRRRRRRRR RRRRRRRRRRRRRRRR RRRRRRRRRRRRRRRR RRRRRRRRRRRRRRRR RRRRRRRRRRRRRRRR The "p" located near the upper right quandrant is exceedingly harder to find. In attempting to find the "P" among "R"s, the process is generally called "serial search" because you have to painstakingly search each row or column to find the P. There is an entire field of psychology devoted to studying these kinds of visual perception phenomena. Anything written by a man named JJ Gibson (scholar.google it) in the realm of visual perception would make a great citation. There are even specific time lapse data available in Gibson's work and others. They have actually calculated the amount of time delay in participant responses based on the type of feature that is absent or present (i.e., contrast, color, etc.). Now then, for the first part of your question. There is a sort of "memory" component, but it's generally not talked about in terms of "memory", but rather is called often called "selective attention." There is neurological research on this subject as well as experimental research, but the whole of it does assert your main point. Think about it and really try to intuit this: if the brain were to selectively attend to everything constantly, we wouldn't be able to really focus on anything with any detail. Generally, neurological research describes perception as an active cyclical process in which we don't really "see" anything except for the things which are changing. Our "visual memory" only lasts about 2, maybe 3 seconds. So, when something changes or is visually stimulating, we look; then our physical bodies (eyes) interpret the stimulus into 'data' our brain can understand, and then either the brain is stimulated and so we take action by look closer (or picking the object up, etc.) or we do nothing. The key here is that if we've already looked at someting with sufficient repetition, it is no longer mentally stimulating (or necessarily effortful) and so then we don't bother looking at it closer any more. Some great neurological research on this subject is in a paper at the national academy of sciences by Desimone in 1996. I think that should sufficiently answer your questions. But to recap a) selective attention b) visual perception |
If you feel that you have found inappropriate content, please let us know by emailing us at answers-support@google.com with the question ID listed above. Thank you. |
Search Google Answers for |
Google Home - Answers FAQ - Terms of Service - Privacy Policy |